Remember, Remember…
Science Writer Competition
for the Daily Telegraph by Tony Clohesy
Everyone remembers the 5th
of November, and most of us can work out how many days in October if we’re
given a few seconds to think. But why
do we find it so difficult to memorize a shopping list? Some people can’t remember their own phone
number while others can recite the first thousand digits of pi. Global competitions are held every year to
see how many shuffled packs of cards can be recalled correctly after only one
sighting. The record is 54 – that’s
over 2,800 cards! Is it all to do with
heredity? Does it depend on the way you
are brought up?
Memory has been a hot topic
as long as anyone can remember; Ancient Greece produced some of the earliest
and most talented memory scientists, or mnemonists. Ever wondered why people say “In the first place…” and “In the
second place…” during speeches?
Apparently it began in Greece and relates to the Locus method of
remembering things: Objects, phrases or ideas are mentally stored in a locus –
a place – such as a house or a palace, and to retrieve them you only need to
work your way mentally through the rooms and pick up what you left. This system works on the principle of
association (“That reminds me…”). The
better the association, the easier it is to remember. ‘Rabid werewolf’ is longer than ‘Lethologica’, but unless you
know what it means, lethologica will be promptly forgotten while the rabid
werewolf will be breathing down your neck when you next think of the principle
of association. The recognition of this
principle is credited to Hedwig von Restorff who published her results in 1933.
The theory of associations
is backed up by the physical structure of the brain. In the ‘gray matter’, the cerebral cortex, millions of synapses
(junctions) link nerves together. When
we make associations of ideas, new synapses are created to link a stimulus in
one part of the brain with others and provoke new responses. Nerves carry information by electrical
charge, doing a similar job to wires in a computer circuit, making synapses the
equivalent of logic gates. Associations
linking the left side (logic, patterns) to the right side (pictures, ideas)
also help.
Image to illustrate how much of the brain is occupied
with memory, arguably its most important function [1]
People
noted for amazing memory take advantage of this fact by making even the most
mundane objects stand out in their mind.
They give them outrageous characteristics, making them large,
ridiculous, or strongly emotive.
Appealing to more than one sense also increases the number of new
associations. If deprived of one sense,
the brain links memories more strongly with another. Helen Keller, struck blind and deaf at age 2 once said:
‘Smell is a potent wizard that transports you across
thousands of miles and all the years you have lived.’
[2]
A Russian called Solomon
Veniaminovich Shereshevsky had an extreme form of the condition known as
synaesthesia which forces vivid visualization of practically everything. Physical stimulation of one sense causes multiple
stimulation of all the others. Whatever
he saw or heard was translated into a complex pattern including sight, sound,
smell, texture and taste. When he heard
a single musical note, he said:
‘It
looks something like fireworks tinged with a pink-red hue. The strip of color feels
rough and unpleasant, and it has an ugly taste -- rather like that of a briny
pickle... You could hurt your hand on this.’
[3]
Reading was difficult for
him because he would visualize things in too much detail, only to have them
contradicted later on even in the same sentence.
Phenomenal memory starting
to sound less desirable? One man who
lost an almost photographic memory after an accident at age ten says:
‘There
are definite advantages to being able to forget certain things.’
[4]
I interviewed Paul Szauter,
a professional mnemonist. He has, among
other things, memorized 5200 digits of pi in a grid in order to recall the
position of any particular ten digit block.
He says:
‘The
core of all of this is translating numbers to letters…Everything is
visualization with action…’
[5]
Paul has a naturally good
memory, but this isn’t always an advantage:
‘We
don’t watch television at all at home, because I can’t get the lousy shows and
stupid characters out of my mind…television seems to be written for people who
aren’t paying attention and who won’t remember it.’
[5]
Many great mnemonists have
expressed similar problems. Alexander
Craig Aitken couldn’t rid himself of the memory of his horrific experience of
the first world war. Although being
able to remember better would be great in some ways, maybe forgetfulness is
just as essential in its own way. By
the way, lethologica is the temporary inability to remember a word – the ‘tip
of the tongue’ phenomenon. Will you
remember that?
[1]
Diagram of the brain used
with permission from the University of Colorado; www.colorado.edu.
[2]
Helen Keller (1880 – 1968)
quotation thanks to www.quotationspage.com.
[3]
The Mind of a Mnemonist: A
Little Book about a Vast Memory, A. R. Luria (trans. L. Solotaroff) Cambridge:
Harvard University Press, 1968.
[4]
Extract from an answer to a
question on memory taken from the public domain of Google Answers; answers.google.com.
[5]
Paul Szauter (www.memoryelixir.com). Many thanks for your comprehensive and
insightful communications.
(789 words excluding captions
& bibliography)